Abstract

Methods

The 14-day study was conducted in 12 public elementary schools in a suburban school district, which follows the offer model allowing students to select either a fruit or vegetable. The percent of students who consumed the target vegetable was measured and compared between 6 experimental and 6 control schools using a logistic regression framework.  Odds ratio tests were used to determine how much more likely a student at an experimental school was to consume the cold vegetable in comparison to a student at a control school.

Results

For all visits, the ratio was significantly larger than one.  The probability of consuming the cold vegetable was 1.4 to 2.9 times greater at the experimental schools.

Applications to Child Nutrition Professionals

When a fresh cold fruit was paired with a cold vegetable as one unit, students in elementary schools significantly increased their consumption of vegetables. Strategies for encouraging students to take healthy options is an effective first step in increasing consumption of these items.

Full Article

With the rise in childhood obesity over the past two decades, public health advocates and nutrition experts have focused on the school environment (Ogden, Carroll, Kit & Flegal, 2014; National Center for Health Statistics, 2011).   Schools have a powerful influence on students’ eating behaviors (Story, Nanney, & Schwartz, 2009), and by changing the school menu and food offerings, there is an opportunity to create a culture of healthful eating practices where children may receive up to three meals daily (Story, Kaphingst, Robinson-O’Brien, & Glanz, 2008). Clearly, since school cafeterias serve an estimated 95% of children and adolescents nationwide, they are regarded as an optimum setting for improving children’s health.

The school food environment is considered a partner in the national efforts to decrease childhood obesity rates (Jaime & Lock, 2009). School nutrition policies may address school meals that meet U.S. Department of Agriculture (USDA) standards and that encourage students to make healthful choices (Briggs, Mueller & Fleischhacker, 2010). At the policy level, the federal government has taken strong steps toward improving childhood nutrition in schools. As required by the Healthy, Hunger-Free Kids Act of 2010 (HHFKA), USDA updated school nutrition standards to align with the 2010 Dietary Guidelines for Americans (USDA, & U. S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2010).  The new standards, which were first implemented in the 2012-2013 school year, focus on increased fruit, vegetable, and whole grain menu offerings, along with a decrease in sodium, high-fat, and sugar-sweetened foods (USDA, 2017).

There are different ways to combat obesity. Higher fruit and vegetable consumption has been linked to lower incidences of obesity (Rolls, Ello-Martin, & Tohill, 2004). Fruit and vegetable consumption leads to overall improved child development, weight management, and higher academic scores (Mikkelsen, Chehimi, & Cohen 2007; Florence, Asbridge, & Veugelers, 2008; Basch, 2011). Unfortunately, children’s consumption of fruits and vegetables is much lower than recommended guidelines (Hockesin, 2010), and furthermore, studies have shown that children from lower socioeconomic status families consume significantly less fruits and vegetables than their counterparts (Rasmussen et al., 2006, Epstein et al., 2001).

Now that healthier options are being offered to children within the school environment, it is critical for health professionals to nudge or influence students to make healthier lunch choices.  The field of behavioral economics combines psychology and economics and assesses strategies that can help shape one’s behavior while being economically efficient.

With healthier options replacing some of the more familiar, typically unhealthy choices to which students may be more accustomed, many school nutrition personnel and school districts are concerned about plate waste.  Plate waste is costly to school districts and the environment, as well as the students, who are missing the opportunity to consume nutrient-rich foods (Cohen et al., 2013).  However, one study determined that the new standards do not lead to more plate waste and students are consuming more fruits than before (Schwartz, Henderson, Read, Danna, & Ickovics, 2015).

Within the school lunch setting, previous behavioral economic strategies that have had success increasing fruit and vegetable consumption include the following: 1) rearranging the food items to make the healthier options more accessible and convenient (Just & Wansink, 2010);

2) attaching stickers of cartoon characters to the fruits/vegetables (Brunt, Bezbaruah, & Stastny, 2012; Belot, Jonathan, & Nolen, 2014); 3) verbal encouragement from cafeteria staff (Schwartz, 2007); and  4) pre-slicing the fruit (Wansink, Just, Hanks, & Smith, 2013).  Furthermore, exposure is a critical component to increasing vegetable consumption among children.  The more a child tastes a novel food, the more he/she will tend to develop a liking for that food.  Studies have shown that children may need 6-10 exposures to a certain food before they start enjoying it (Anzman-Frasca, Savage, Marini, Fisher, & Birch,2012; Lakkakula, Geaghan, Zanovec, Pierce, & Tuuri, 2010; Williams, Paul, Pizzo, & Riegel, 2008; Wardle et al., 2003).  Other studies have shown that the use of small, repeated taste tests could increase vegetable consumption among children (Lakkakula, et al., 2010; Cooke, 2007).

Behavioral economics suggests that the appearance of choice in making decisions about a certain behavior can lead to a positive association with the outcome (Hakim & Meissen, 2013; Wansink, 2013). Choosing among a variety of healthful foods in the cafeteria setting can teach students how to make healthy decisions outside of school as well.  Hakim et al. (2013) determined that when students are served a meal with a fruit and then given the choice of selecting between multiple vegetables, or vice versa, students will consume significantly higher amounts of both fruits and vegetables.

In 1981, the policy of “offer versus serve” was created under the National School Lunch Program to promote a well-balanced diet while reducing plate waste and improving efficiency for the cafeteria operations.   The traditional “serve” policy required students to take all of the served meal components. Due to concerns over plate waste, schools were allowed to follow an offer vs. serve model which allowed students to choose three out of five meal components: grain, protein, fruit, vegetable, and milk (HHFKA, 2010). More recently, that policy was updated so that students are required to take three components including ½ cup of a fruit or vegetable to qualify for a reimbursable meal (USDA-FNS, 2015).

Past research indicates that student consumption of fruit is consistently higher than vegetables. Fox, Clark, Condon, and Wilson (2009) used the Healthy Eating Index 2005 to assess diet quality of elementary school children compared to the 2005 U.S. Dietary Guidelines.  They reported that fruit intake was 82% of the maximum possible score compared to vegetable intake at 38%.  Further, dark green and orange vegetables had a very low score of 10%.  This indicates that students tend to select a fruit over a vegetable in the cafeteria line and are not likely to consume dark green and orange vegetables that provide different nutrients compared to fruit.

In addressing the childhood obesity epidemic, the new school lunch program standards and behavioral economic research are creating cafeteria environments that influence children to select and consume healthier foods.  Yet, vegetable consumption is very low among elementary school children and a higher intake of fruit and vegetable is inversely related to obesity.  Novel approaches to introducing students to dark green and orange vegetables are needed in the school cafeteria. One novel approach is to pair fruits and cold vegetables as one unit.  Placing both the fruit and vegetable on the students’ trays may encourage consumption of both items.

This current study evaluated a hybrid version of the “offer versus serve” model in a school district that normally uses the offer model. The goal was to increase vegetable consumption without displacing fruit consumption. Generally, students would have a choice of selecting the fruit, cold vegetable, or hot vegetable.  In this case, students had the choice of selecting the hot vegetable and/or a fruit/vegetable combination that included a serving of fresh fruit and a cold vegetable packaged together in a clear, pint-sized bag with a sticker on the front. The hypothesis was that the pairing technique would increase the percent of students who take and consume a cold vegetable while not decreasing consumption of the fruit.

 METHODOLOGY

Participants

The study was conducted in grades Pre-K-5 in 12 public elementary schools in a suburban school district during Spring 2015.  Data collectors tracked fruit and vegetable consumption over 14 days, on Tuesdays and Wednesdays, for seven non-consecutive weeks, compiling a total of 33,781 student-meal observations.  Baseline data were collected for two days in all schools, and consumption was measured for each cold vegetable, hot vegetable, and fruit. The 12 public elementary schools followed the standards issued by USDA that require all schools offer a fruit and vegetable daily for lunch. The school district utilizes an offer model; therefore, students are required to take either a fruit or a vegetable in order to qualify for a reimbursable meal. Students who pay full price for meals are not bound by this provision.

Study Design

The technique of pairing a fresh cold fruit with a cold vegetable was used in six experimental schools.  The fruit and cold vegetable were packaged together in pint-sized, clear, plastic bags. Examples of a paired fruit and vegetable include an apple with baby carrots or an orange with pepper strips. Attractive, age-appropriate stickers were placed on each bag to entice students to take the bags.  Students were allowed to make the choice of selecting the packaged bag and/or the hot vegetable; however, individual servings of fruit or cold vegetables were not available at the experimental schools.  Schools were assigned to be control or experimental at random and remained under this classification for the entirety of the study. The schools were comparable in a number of ways: student enrollment, demographics, geographic location, percent free and reduced-price lunch eligibility, academic proficiency scores, and National School Lunch Program participation rates (Table 1).

Consumption data were collected using a smartphone application called “V-Project,” developed by Dr. Joseph Price at Brigham Young University (Brigham Young University, 2015).   The application allows the data collector to input school information, and non-identifying student information such as grade and gender and the entrée for the day, as well as individual items served.  Consumption of these items is measured on a sliding scale from 0-100 %. For the current study, data collectors recorded each student’s fruit and vegetable consumption.

Study Procedures

Data collectors were stationed in the cafeteria during the entire lunch period. Once a student finished his or her lunch and approached the waste bin, the data collectors recorded: 1) whether the student took the packaged cold fruit and vegetable, and 2) the amounts consumed of both the fruit and vegetable.  Data was collected on consumption of each individual fruit and vegetable. If the student selected the fruit or vegetable, but did not eat the fruit and/or vegetable, it was recorded as “0 %.” Any consumption greater than zero (i.e. one bite) but less than the entirety of the portion was marked as “50 %,” and full completion of the serving was marked as “100 %.”  Therefore, consumption was defined as consuming 50-100 % of the food item. In previous research, this half-waste method yielded the best inter-method reliability for visual measurement with a proven .83 reliability measure of vegetables based on three vegetable food items (Hanks, Wansink, & Just, 2014).  Using the app, data collectors noted students who did not take a fruit or vegetable, and these students were recorded as zero consumption. Data collectors were trained on the V-project application prior to the study including verbal instructions and hands-on practice with the application for visually measuring consumption. Two data collectors were assigned to each school.

Following baseline collection, the fruit and cold vegetable were bagged together in a clear, plastic bag with an attractive sticker by the school nutrition staff at the experimental schools. The bags were presented in the same location where the fruits and vegetables would normally be served in the lunch line. This pairing occurred twice per week (on Tuesday and Wednesday) for four consecutive weeks, followed by a week break for spring break, and then again for two consecutive weeks.  On average, approximately 300 bags were used at each experimental school during each intervention visit.  In the control schools, fruits and vegetables were offered as usual.

Table 1.  Description of All Experimental and Control Schools

 

School Name

Total

Students

Average Student Meal Observations Total School Meal Observations  

Demographics

Free/Reduced Rate

(%)

Breakfast Participation (%) Lunch Participation (%)
Experimental            
School E-1 570 242 3,389 Hispanic (42.9%); White (29.8%); Black (13.1%) 50.7 18.4 54.0
School E-2 549 252 3,524 Hispanic (44.9%); White (36.0%); Black (8.9%) 55.6 19.3 59.2
School E-3 729 193 2,706 Hispanic (52.3%); White (36.2%); Black (5.0%) 35.0 11.3 52.8
School E-4 406 170 2,380 Black (39.8%); Hispanic (30.3%); Asian (16.2%); 65.0 21.8 60.9
School E-5 678 205 2,867 Hispanic (52.2%); White (33.3%); Black (5.6%) 41.9 15.2 58.0
School E-6 700 211 2,954  White (43.8%); Black (18.8%; Hispanic (16.4%) 29.6 11.4 49.2
Control              
School C-1 568 181 2,353 White (61.4%); Hispanic (13.8%); Asian (11.3%) 20.8 8.6 37.9
School C-2 498 228 3,192 Hispanic (48.8%); White (25.7%); Asian (10.8%) 59.6 25.8 61.7
School C-3 424 151 2,110 Hispanic (39.3%); White (30.6%); Asian (14.3%) 56.6 34.8 65.2
School C-4 638 185 2,586 Hispanic (35.1%); Black (31.9%); White (20.6%) 58.0. 19.8 66.3
School C-5 565 184 2,580 White (64.4%); Hispanic (15.0%); Asian (8.4%) 17.9 6.4 41.5

 

School C-6 429 224 3,140 Hispanic (55.6%); Black (21.1%); Asian (11.9%) 77.2 19.5 74.2

Data collection methods at all twelve follow-up visits were identical to baseline measurement.  Consumption data were aggregated in the same manner and compared to baseline to assess the effect of the intervention.   Odds ratios comparing the odds of vegetable consumption (50% or 100% versus none) in the experimental group to the control group were estimated at each baseline and follow-up time using the logistic regression formulation of the two-way contingency table. With large sample sizes and independent assignment of treatment groups, researchers used the asymptotic normal distribution of the coefficient estimate for hypothesis tests and confidence intervals. The university’s Institutional Review Board approved this study.

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

Estimates of the odds ratio for the baseline visits provided evidence that the students were less likely to consume the cold vegetable at the experimental schools (Table 2; odds ratio < 1). For all follow-up visits, the tests showed evidence that the ratio is significantly larger than one, supporting the effect of the experiment and specifically estimating that the probability of consuming the cold vegetable when packaged with a cold fruit was 1.4 to 2.9 times greater at the experimental schools. Individual model fit assessments using the Hosmer-Lemeshow goodness of fit test gave no evidence of model deficiencies.

Table 2.  Estimated Odds Ratios of Vegetable Consumption Comparing Experimental to Control Schools from Separate Logistic Regression Models
Visit Odds Ratio 95% Confidence Interval p-values
Baseline 1 0.67 (0.51, 0.95) .011
Baseline 2 0.50 (0.39, 0.65) <.001
Follow-up 1 2.44 (1.91, 3.14) <.001
Follow-up 2 1.78 (1.42, 2.23) <.001
Follow-up 3 1.37 (1.06, 1.79) .010
Follow-up 4 1.52 (1.19, 1.95) .001
Follow-up 5 1.41 (1.04, 1.93) .015
Follow-up 6 1.59 (1.29, 1.97) <.001
Follow-up 7 1.69 (1.32, 2.17) .001
Follow-up 8 1.47 (1.16, 1.86) .001
Follow-up 9 2.88 (2.23, 3.75) <.001
Follow-up 10 1.78 (1.37, 2.30) <.001
Follow-up 11 2.35 (1.73, 3.20) <.001
Follow-up 12 1.41 (1.10, 1.81) .001
*Odds ratio = the estimated odds of vegetable consumption at visit time for experimental group divided by the odds of vegetable consumption at visit time for control group.

+ The p-value tests for differences in the odds of vegetable consumption between the experimental and control groups.

Additionally, for each school, average cold vegetable consumption was calculated for comparison between baseline and follow-up visits.  This process was also repeated for fruit consumption.  Tables 3 and 4 display the average consumption of cold vegetables and fruits at each of the schools for baseline and follow-up visits.  When baseline and follow-up percentages were compared at control schools, cold vegetable consumption did not tend to increase and stayed fairly consistent for five of the six schools. At the experimental schools, cold vegetable consumption increased substantially at four of the six schools.  For the two other schools, cold vegetable consumption slightly increased at one and slightly decreased at the other (5-E). The slight decrease at this school may be attributed to the fact that additional fruit was available in a fruit bowl on the line in addition to the pairing technique.

Table 3.  Percent of Students Consuming Vegetables and Fruit at the Control Schools
  Cold Vegetable Fruit
School Average Baseline

(%)

Average Follow-Up

(%)

Average

Baseline

(%)

Average

Follow-Up

(%)

C-1 11.85 11.81 32.22 37.06
C-2 6.72 6.32 31.38 43.22
C-3 23.68 9.39 30.41 39.20
C-4 14.48 16.23 35.52 46.78
C-5 12.63 12.83 36.29 34.38
C-6 10.44 9.25 67.36 61.08
Table 4.  Percent of Students Consuming Vegetable and Fruit Consumed at the Experimental Schools
  Cold Vegetable Fruit
 School Average

 Baseline

(%)

Average

Follow-Up

(%)

Average

 Baseline

(%)

Average

Follow-Up

(%)

E-1 4.30 18.51 54.12 42.85
E-2 13.54 20.14 54.75 55.66
E-3 5.76 20.14 45.68 48.11
E-4 6.44 16.27 53.66 48.50
E-5 13.03 11.76 29.91 36.95
E-6 9.09 11.41 40.26 32.15

Graph 1 displays the percentage of students from all experimental schools and control schools who tasted or ate all of the cold vegetable by the week of the intervention.  Cold vegetable consumption spiked at experimental schools during the first week of intervention and then declined; however, vegetable consumption still remained higher throughout the entirety of the study at experimental schools in comparison to the control schools.

Graph 2 displays cold vegetable and fruit consumption at experimental schools for all 14 days of the study.  While cold vegetable consumption had an upward trend from baseline to follow-up, fruit consumption did not decrease; therefore, higher consumption of the cold vegetables did not

Results indicate that the packaging technique significantly increased cold vegetable consumption at the experimental schools compared to the control schools while not changing the consumption of fruit. The spike in cold vegetable consumption at experimental schools during the first week could be expected; the bags with stickers were new to the students and possibly influenced their desire to take the package. However, it is noteworthy that the cold vegetable consumption was

nearly twice as high in each of the follow-up weeks at the experimental schools compared to the baseline week.  Additionally, the increased consumption of the cold vegetable at the experimental schools did not replace or reduce the fruit consumption significantly.

At one of the experimental schools (E-5) a “fruit bowl” was an option in addition to the bagged fruit and vegetable pair for 6 of the 12 days of the intervention. The fruit bowl contained different fruits from which students could select a fruit choice. Data for the 6 days from this school were discarded as a result.

During the follow-up periods, there were some limitations on a visit-by-visit basis.  For Follow-up 5, there was no cold vegetable served at two of the control schools (C-5 and C-6), and at one of the experimental schools (E-3) the fruit and cold vegetable were not bagged. On Follow up 6, no hot vegetable was served in 11 of the 12 schools.  Follow-up 10 was an early-release day for all schools in the district, which made for a shorter lunch period.

 CONCLUSIONS AND APPLICATION

 This approach of pairing a fruit and vegetable in the school cafeteria line showed a significant increase in cold vegetable consumption. Previous research has shown fruit consumption to be higher than vegetable consumption for elementary school children (Fox, et al., 2009); therefore, pairing a food that students regularly select with a food that they rarely select may be an effective strategy for increasing consumption of dark green and orange vegetables. The concept behind behavioral economics is to nudge or influence students to select healthful foods.  When a fruit was paired with a cold vegetable in this study, students were not forced to select between the items, and with both items on their tray, consumption of vegetables significantly increased.

While the packaging technique significantly increased cold vegetable consumption, this method requires more time and labor from the cafeteria manager and staff; however, the results of this study can pave the way for more practical opportunities.  Changes can be made at the policy level, where students are required to take a fruit and a vegetable.  In that case, they are still empowered to make a choice between the hot and cold vegetables.  Another possibility is requiring the school meal vendor to pre-package the fruit and vegetable together, although this could increase costs.

In a review of potential determinants of fruit and vegetable intake, Blanchette and Brug (2006) stated that a multi-level approach is the best way to increase fruit and vegetable consumption.  In addition to changes made within the cafeteria setting, incorporating nutrition education, a parent-home element, and computer programs, can have a positive impact (Blanchette & Brug, 2006).  This approach is costly and not practical to incorporate into a majority of schools in our country; therefore, according to French and Stables (2006), the most cost-effective component, which can have a bigger impact on a larger population, is making small changes to the cafeteria environment to nudge children towards healthier choices.

A child’s dietary habits are complex in nature; there are many factors that can influence a child’s food choice.  Having recess before lunch has been found to significantly increase fruit and vegetable consumption (Price & Just, 2015).  Time is another factor that can have an effect on fruit and vegetable consumption.  A recent study determined that students who had more time to eat had significantly increased consumption of the milk, entrée, and vegetable (Cohen et al., 2015). Additionally, a school’s health culture, which can consist of nutrition and physical activity posters, lunch monitor and teacher encouragement, and quality of food, can strongly influence a student’s lifestyle.  In this study, the large sample size and significant increases in vegetable consumption are important outcomes. Evaluating various behavioral economic strategies within an elementary school cafeteria, such as this packaging technique, can help inform future interventions in combatting the childhood obesity epidemic.

There are limitations to the data included in this study. The V-project app experienced technical difficulties; some data collectors recorded data manually for one day and entered it into the app at a later date.  Additionally, much variation existed among the fruit and vegetable items served from school to school. While a district-wide menu is available, the cafeteria manager may serve different or additional items depending on foods available in the kitchen.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The U.S. Department of Agriculture Economic Research Service provided grant funding for this project and collaborated to design the study, which was carried out by an American University research team.  We would also like to thank Arlington Food Service Director, Amy Maclosky, and the foodservice team at Arlington Public Schools in Arlington, VA.  The views expressed are solely those of the authors and should not be attributed to the Economic Research Service or the U.S. Department of Agriculture.

References

Anzman-Frasca, S., Savage, J.S., Marini, M.E., Fisher, J.O., & Birch, L.L. (2012). Repeated exposure and associative conditioning promote preschool children’s liking of vegetables. Appetite, 58(2),543-553. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2011.11.012

Basch, C.E. (2011). Healthier students are better learners: A missing link in school reforms to close the achievement gap. Journal of  School Health, 81(10), 593 -598.

doi:10.1111/j.1746-1561.2011.00632.x

Belot, M., Jonathan, J., & Nolen, P. (2014). Incentives and children’s dietary choices: A field experiment in primary schools. Scottish Institute for Research in Economics Discussion Paper No. 41226. Retrieved March 20, 2017 from http://repo.sire.ac.uk/bitstream/handle/10943/471/SIRE-DP-2013-44.pdf?sequence=1&isAllowed=y

Blanchette, L.,  & Brug, G. (2006). Determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among 6–12-year-old children and effective interventions to increase consumption. International Journal of Behavior, Nutrition & Physical Activity, 3(22), 431-443. doi:10.1186/1479-5868-3-22

Brigham Young University. (2015). The Veggie Project: Data collection. Retrieved March 20, 2017 from https://veggieproject.byu.edu/Pages/index.aspx

Briggs, M., Mueller, C., & Fleischhacker, S. (2010). Position of the American Dietetic Association, School Nutrition Association, and Society for Nutrition Education: Comprehensive school nutrition services. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 110, 1738-1749.

doi:10.1016/j.jada.2010.08.035

Brunt, A., Bezbaruah, N., & Stastny, S. (2012). Do spokes-characters improve consumption of vegetables among children?  Journal of Nutrition Education & Behavior, 44(4),S77-S78. doi:10.1016/j.jneb.2012.03.182

Cohen, J., John, J., Richardson, S., Cluggish, S., Parker, E., & Rimm, E. (2015).  Amount of time to eat lunch is associated with children’s selection and consumption of school meal entrée, fruits, vegetables, and milk. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition & Dietetics, 116(1), 123-128.   http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jand.2015.07

Cooke, L. (2007). The importance of exposure for healthy eating in childhood: A review. Journal of Human Nutrition & Dietetics, 20(4), 294-301. doi:10.1111/j.1365-277X.2007.00804.x

Epstein, L.H., Gordy, C.C., Raynor, H.A., Beddome,  M., Kilanowski, C.K., & Paluch, R. (2001). Increasing fruit and vegetable intake and decreasing fat and sugar intake in families at risk for childhood obesity. Obesity Research, 9(3), 171-178. doi:10.1038/oby.2001

Florence, M., Asbridge, M., & Veugelers P. (2008). Diet quality and academic performance. Journal of School Health, 78(4), 209-215. doi:10.1111/j.1746-1561.2008.00288.x

Fox, M.K., Clark, M., Condon, E., & Wilson, A. (2009). Mathematica Policy Research, Inc.  Diet quality of school-age children in the U.S. and associations with participation in the school meal program. Retrieved March 20, 2017 from https://naldc.nal.usda.gov/download/39789/PDF

French, S., & Stables, G. (2006, December). Environmental interventions to promote vegetable and fruit consumption among youth in school settings. Preventive Medicine, 7(6), 593-610. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2003.09.007

Hakim, S.M., & Meissen, G. (2013). Increasing consumption of fruits and vegetables in the school cafeteria: The influence of active choice. Journal of Health Care for the Poor & Underserved, 24(2),145-57. doi:10.1353/hpu.2013.0109

Hanks, A., Wansink, B., & Just, D. (2014). Reliability and accuracy of real-time visualization techniques for measuring school cafeteria tray waste: Validating the quarter-waste method. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition & Dietetics, 114(3),470-474. doi:10.1016/j.jand.2013.08.013

Healthy, Hunger-Free Kids Act of 2010, (2010). Pub L No. 111-296.

Hockesin, D.E. (2010). Produce for Better Health Foundation. State of the plate: 2010 study on America’s consumption of fruits and vegetables. Retrieved March 20, 2017 from http://www.pbhfoundation.org/pdfs/about/res/pbh_res/stateplate.pdf

Jamie, P.C., & Lock, K. (2008). Do school based food and nutrition policies improve diet and reduce obesity? Preventive Medicine,48 (1), 45-53. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2008.10.018

Just, D.R., & Wansink, B. (2010). Smarter lunchrooms: Using behavioral economics to improve meal selection choices. Retrieved March 20, 2017 from http://foodpsychology.cornell.edu/sites/default/files/unmanaged_files/smarter-lunchrooms.pdf.

Lakkakula A., Geaghan J., Zanovec M., Pierce S., & Tuuri G. (2010). Repeated taste exposure increases liking for vegetables by low-income elementary school children. Appetite, 55(2), 226-231. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2010.06.003

Mikkelsen, L., Chehimi, S., & Cohen, L. (2007). Healthy eating and physical activity: Addressing inequities in urban environments. Oakland, CA: Prevention Institute. Retrieved March 20, 2017 from http://www.preventioninstitute.org/component/jlibrary/article/id-95/127.html

National Center for Health Statistics. Health, United States, 2011: With special features on socioeconomic status and health. Hyattsville, MD; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services; 2012.

Ogden, C.L., Carroll, M.D., Kit, B.K., & Flegal, K.M. (2014). Prevalence of childhood and adult obesity in the United States, 2011-2012. Journal of the American Medical Association, 311(8),806-814. doi:10.1097/01.sa.0000451505.72517.a5

Price, J., &  Just, D.R. (2015). Lunch, recess and nutrition: Responding to time incentives in the cafeteria. Preventive Medicine, 71,27-30.  http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2014.11.016

Rasmussen, M., Krolner, R., Klepp, K., Lytle, L., Brug, J., Bere, E., & Due, P. (2006). Determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among children and adolescents: A review of the literature.  Part I: quantitative studies.  International Journal of Behavior, Nutrition, & Physical Activity, 11(3),22.

Rolls, B.J., Ello-Martin, J.A., & Tohill, B.C. (2004). What can intervention studies tell us about the relationship between fruit and vegetable consumption and weight management? Nutrition Review, 62(1),1-17.

Schwartz, M.B. (2007). The influence of a verbal prompt on school lunch fruit consumption: A pilot study. International Journal of Behavior, Nutrition & Physical Activity, 4(1),6. doi:10.1186/1479-5868-4-6

Schwartz, M.B., Henderson, K.E., Read, M., Danna, N., & Ickovics, J.R. (2015). New school meal regulations increase fruit consumption and do not increase total plate waste. Childhood Obesity, 11(3),242-247. doi:10.1089/chi.2015.0019

Story, M.,  Kaphingst, K.M., Robinson-O’Brien, R., & Glanz, K. (2008). Creating healthy food and eating environments: Policy and environmental approaches. Annual Review of Public Health, 29,253-272. doi:10.1146/annurev.publhealth.29.020907.090926

Story, M., Nanney, M. S., & Schwartz, M. B. (2009). Schools and obesity prevention: Creating school environments and policies to promote healthy eating and physical activity. The Milbank Quarterly87(1), 71–100. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0009.2009.00548.x

U.S. Department of Agriculture, Food and Nutrition Service.  (2015). Offer versus Serve Guidance for the National School Lunch Program and the School Breakfast Program.  Retrieved from https://www.fns.usda.gov/sites/default/files/cn/SP41-2015av2.pdf

U.S. Department of Agriculture, Food and Nutrition Service. (2017). School meals, nutrition standards for school meals. Retrieved from https://www.fns.usda.gov/school-meals/nutrition-standards-school-meals

U.S. Department of Agriculture, & U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (2010). Dietary Guidelines for Americans 2010.  Washington D.C. :U.S. Government Printing Office.

Wansink, B. (2013). Convenient, attractive, and normative.  The CAN approach to making children slim by design. Childhood Obesity, 9(4),227-278. doi:10.1089/chi.2013.9405

Wansink, B., Just, D.R., Hank, A.S., & Smith, L.E. (2013). Pre-sliced fruit in school cafeterias: Children’s selection and intake.  American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 44 (5), 477-480.

Wardle, J., Cooke, L.J., Gibson, E.L., Sapochnik, M., Sheiham, A., & Lawson, M. (2003). Increasing children’s acceptance of vegetables; A randomized trial of parent-led exposure. Appetite, 40 (2):155-162. doi:10.1016/S0195-6663(02)00135-6

Williams, K.E., Paul, C., Pizzo, B., & Riegel, K. (2008). Practice does make perfect. A longitudinal look at repeated taste exposure. Appetite. 51 (3):739-742.

doi:10.1016/j.appet.2008.05.063

Biography

Snelling, Watts, Van Dyke, Malloy, and Ghamarian are all associated with American University located in Washington, DC.  Snelling is Professor and Chair of the Department of Health Studies while Watts is a Research Manager and Ghamarian a Research Associate in the same Department.  Van Dyke is a Lecturer and Malloy an Associate Professor in the Department of Mathematics and Statistics at American University.  Newman, Guthrie, and Mancino are all employed by the Economic Research Services at the U.S. Department of Agriculture in Washington, DC where Newman is an Economist, Guthrie a Research Nutritionist, and Mancino a Research Economist.

Purpose / Objectives

This study evaluated a behavioral economic strategy which paired a fresh cold fruit and cold vegetable to increase consumption of vegetables among elementary school children.