Abstract

Methods

This study utilized a one-group, pretest-posttest design.  The children were offered a combined intervention featuring nutrition education and exposure to novel fruits and vegetables at meals in a preschool setting.

Results

Nineteen children aged 3.3 through 5.6 years completed the study. Children were able to identify several novel fruits and vegetables more accurately following the intervention. The mean calculated scores for identificationpreferencehealthfulness, and exposure all significantly changed from pre- to post-measures (< .05).  BMI z-score was inversely related to identification (r = -.60), preference (r =-.46), healthfulness (r = -.66), and exposure (r = -.49) scores following the intervention (< .05) but not before the intervention.

Application to Child Nutrition Professionals

Children in this pilot study who learned about fruits and vegetables were more likely to identify them as healthy and want to eat them, so exposure at meals in combination with a nutrition education program may improve acceptance of novel food on the school’s menu.

Full Article

Please note that this study was published before the SY2014-15 implementation of the Smart Snacks Nutrition Standards for Competitive Food in Schools, as required by the Healthy, Hunger-Free Kids Acts of 2010. As such, certain research relating to food in schools may not be relevant today.

Obesity prevention is a public health priority, and schools are at the forefront of the fight against childhood obesity in the U.S.  The recent changes to the National School Lunch Program (NSLP) and School Breakfast Program (SBP) illustrate this role (U.S. Department of Agriculture [USDA], 2012).  The rate of obesity among children in the U.S. is approaching 17% (Ogden, Carroll, Kit, & Flegal, 2012).  Although some states are seeing rates decline (Robert Wood Johnson Foundation, 2012), most are not.  Obese children are at increased psychosocial and health risk compared with their normal weight peers (Lobstein, Baur, & Uauy, 2004), and excessive weight in childhood places individuals at higher risk for obesity during adolescence and adulthood (Juonala et al., 2011; Starc & Sterl, 2010). Therefore, understanding which interventions are most effective to prevent obesity among preschool (3-5 years of age) children is critical not only to curb but also to reverse obesity rates.

A key area of obesity prevention efforts has included dietary education and dietary behavior modification in the elementary school or after-school setting (Sigman-Grant et al., 2011).  Older children do not always eat the healthful foods on their lunch trays (Baik & Lee, 2009; Cashman, Tripuranan, Englund, & Bergman, 2010); however, recent research has demonstrated the effectiveness of modifying elementary school cafeteria environments to improve nutrition (Williams, Han, Johnson, Martin, & Newton, 2013).  While improving diets among older children is important, some studies suggest the need for interventions before children reach elementary school age (Harnack et al., 2012), and less is known about optimizing healthy eating in preschool child care and education settings.

Preschools can modify the environment to attempt to optimize healthful eating.  Understanding how preschoolers respond to menu changes is critical, as future changes to the Child and Adult Care Food Program (CACFP) may be informed by additional research in this area.  Although dietary recommendations to eat more fruits and vegetables for good health and reduced obesity risk are not new, less is known about the effectiveness of teaching preschool-aged children about the benefits of healthful foods and exposing them to new fruits and vegetables at mealtimes.  The purpose of this pilot study was to compare changes in preschool children’s identification, preferences, and beliefs related to fruits and vegetables introduced to a child care center’s menu before and after a nutrition education and food exposure intervention.  The study also sought to determine how these changes were related to Body Mass Index (BMI).

Methodology

Participant Characteristics
Participants included children enrolled at a university preschool located in the Upper Midwest region of the United States. Parents of recruited children were informed of the detailed procedures of the study and were provided with options to allow their child to participate or opt out without any consequences. All study protocols were approved by the university’s institutional review board prior to initiation of the pilot study.

Design and Procedure
This one-group, pretest-posttest study evaluated preschool children’s responses to novel fruits and vegetables offered at meals in a child care setting and simultaneous exposure to a nutrition education program featuring these new foods. Study participants were introduced to six new food items: blueberries, sugar snap peas, raspberries, kiwi, winter squash, and jicama.  The center’s rotating menu was modified by serving one new fruit or vegetable in addition to the regular menu items at breakfast, lunch, and snack meals during the intervention.  Each of the six new fruits and vegetables were offered eight times over a four-week period.  Storage, cost, variety, and presentation were all important considerations when novel foods were chosen for the study.  Therefore, kiwi and jicama were served fresh, frozen blueberries and raspberries were served partially thawed, and frozen winter squash and sugar snap peas were steamed to demonstrate a range of presentation options.

A nutrition educator offered participants four 30-minute lessons which were held on a weekly basis to introduce the six novel fruits and vegetables.  The educator utilized an adapted version of the Color Me Healthy! Curriculum (Dunn, Thomas, Pegram, Ward, & Schmal, 2004). A detailed description of the methods utilized for the education program has been published elsewhere (Niemeier, Tande, Hwang, Stastny, & Hektner, 2010).

Data were collected through structured interviews appropriate for the ages of participants. The interview questions were reviewed by two developmental psychologists to ensure that the questions were developmentally appropriate for preschool children.  The interviews were designed to assess children’s ability to identify foods and to measure their preferences for and beliefs about the foods presented to them. Researchers presented seven food items during the interview in the following order, selected by random drawing, at both pre- and post-intervention: blueberries, soda pop, winter squash, sugar snap peas, jicama, candy, and kiwi.  For each food, study participants were asked “What is this?”, “Is it healthy?”, and “Have you ever eaten this?”  If they responded that they had eaten a food, they were asked two follow-up questions: “Did you like it?” and “Would you eat it again?”  If they responded that they had not eaten a food, they were asked “If we had some for lunch or supper or snack, would you eat it?”  Soda pop and candy were included for two reasons: to assess preschool children’s ability to identify healthy vs. unhealthy foods and to assess reliability of the interview questions.  Five of the six novel fruits and vegetables were included in the interview, with raspberries omitted due to the length of the interview.

Responses were coded one of two ways: correct responses and “yes” as 1; incorrect responses, no response, and “no” as 0.  One point was tabulated for a correct answer related to each fruit or vegetable for the following categories (scores ranged from 0 – 5):
Identification – identifying fruits and vegetables
Healthfulness – identifying fruits and vegetables as healthy foods
Exposure – reporting previously eating the fruits and vegetables
Preference – reporting “liking” fruits and vegetables
Exploration – reporting willingness to eat fruits and vegetables at future meals or snacks
The scale for each score was 0 to 5 with 5 being the maximum score possible for correct or “yes” responses for all 5 fruits and vegetables in each category.

Heights and weights of the children were measured at both pre- and post-intervention as part of the interview protocol, and BMI, BMI z-score, and BMI percentile were calculated. Childhood overweight and obesity were defined as = 85th percentile and = 95th percentile, respectively (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2012).

Nonparametric, related samples tests were conducted to measure changes in distributions of values for identification, preference, and belief for each interviewed item from pre- to post-intervention.  Correlations (Spearman’s Rho, nonparametric) were utilized to evaluate relationships between BMI z-score and identification, preference, and belief scores pre- and post-intervention.  Dependent t-tests identified changes in identification, preference, and belief scores from pre- to post-intervention.  Data were analyzed in SPSS, version 18 (IBM, Armonk, NY).

Results And Discussion

Nineteen children, aged 3.3 through 5.6 years (= 4.6 years, SD = .63), completed this pilot study.  Fifteen of 19 children were White with 11 girls and 8 boys completing the study. Eight of the 19 and 7 of the 19 participants were overweight or obese at the beginning, and at the end of the study, respectively. The mean BMI z-score did not significantly change from the beginning of the study to the conclusion of the study (= .758, SD = .849 and = .710, SD = .927, respectively).

Children were able to identify several novel fruits and vegetables more accurately following the intervention, including winter squash, sugar snap peas, jicama, and kiwi (< .001, = .002, = .002,= .001, respectively) (Table 1).  More children also reported having eaten jicama and liking it (< .001) and “liking” sugar snap peas, which approached significance (= .06).  Following the intervention, preschool children were more likely to report that winter squash was a “healthy” food (p= .031) and that they had eaten the food (= .039).

BMI z-score was not correlated with any of the interview scores at baseline. However, at the conclusion of the study, the baseline BMI z-score was significantly inversely related to the post-measures of identificationpreference, healthfulness, and exposure (df = 18, < .05 for all measures), indicating the lower children’s baseline BMI, the higher their scores were, on average, for identificationhealthfulnessexposure, and preference at the conclusion of the study.  The relationships between BMI z-scores and the change in scores from pre- to post-intervention were also evaluated with baseline BMI inversely related to change in exposure (= -.512,  = .025) and preference (= -.468, = .043) scores from pre- to post-intervention. Study results suggest that BMI status predicted changes in trying new fruits and vegetables and liking them.

 Discussion
This pilot study is the first to report data gathered by interviewing preschool-aged children individually and directly about their fruit and vegetable identification, preference, and beliefs before and after exposure to novel fruits and vegetables at meals accompanied by nutrition education lessons.  When examining the new fruits and vegetables collectively, children’s ability to identify them improved.  Children who accurately identified these foods were also more likely to report them as healthy foods, that they liked these foods, and that they had eaten them before.  When examining the foods on an individual basis, however, preference increased for only jicama, suggesting that more exposures may be needed or a larger sample to identify changes in individual food acceptability.

The findings in this pilot study suggest that how preschool children respond to exposure and education could be correlated with BMI status.  Children with more favorable BMI statuses may be more likely to accept novel foods than their counterparts from similar interventions. Thus, those preschool children at highest risk of obesity and overweight or who currently have BMI percentiles in these categories may need additional education or fruit and vegetable exposures, or perhaps, they may benefit from a different intervention strategy altogether. Limited research has compared the response of normal weight and overweight children to dietary interventions.

Limitations of this study include a small sample size of a relatively homogenous group of preschool children and the lack of a control group.   An important future study would include control and intervention groups with a larger, more diverse sample. The intervention would require 20 exposures for each fruit and vegetable new to the child care menu along with nutrition education. Measures including heights, weights, food beliefs, food intake, and food costs would be collected at baseline, 6 months, and 12 months.

Conclusions And Application

Early childhood is a critical time for development of food likes and dislikes and a period of opportunity to impact current and future health (Northstone & Emmett, 2008).  Schools remain at the forefront of the childhood obesity fight, and preschools can modify the environment to optimize healthful eating.  The recent changes to the NSLP and SBP (USDA, 2012) emphasize the importance of learning more about how preschool children and toddlers (1-3 years of age) respond to menu changes and nutrition education to optimize food acceptability and minimize food waste.  The fruits and vegetables in this study were chosen because of their long-term viability on preschool child care menus.  Other practical menu choices include pre-cut finger foods, fresh lettuce and spinach, frozen fruit snacks, thawed frozen peas (not cooked), dried fruit, and chickpea-based dips such as hummus.  Some fresh foods are cost-prohibitive so frozen (sugar snap peas, squash, mixed vegetables), dried (cranberries, blueberries, apricots, bananas), and canned (pumpkin, oranges, fusion 100% juice drinks) foods can offer variety and options all year.

Understanding how preschoolers respond to menu changes is critical, as future changes to the CACFP may be informed by additional research in this area with a larger, more diverse sample with financial costs measured and potential benefits estimated.  This information is important to inform both current and future practices in foodservice management, nutrition education, and behavior change.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the parents, children, and child care providers who participated and supported this research project.  No competing financial interests exist.

References

Baik, J-Y., & Lee, H.  (2009). Habitual plate-waste of 6- to 9-year-olds may not be associated with lower nutritional needs or taste acuity, but undesirable dietary factors.  Nutrition Research, 29(12), 831-838. doi:10.1016/j.nutres.2009.10.009

Cashman, L., Tripuranan, M., Englund, T., & Bergman, E. A. (2010). Food group preferences of elementary school children participating in the National School Lunch Program.  Journal of Child Nutrition & Management, 34(1). Retrieved from
http://www.schoolnutrition.com/jcnm

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2012). Overweight and obesity: Basics about childhood obesity. Retrieved from  http://www.cdc.gov/obesity/childhood/basics.html

Dunn C., Thomas C., Pegram L., Ward D., & Schmal S. (2004). Color me healthy, preschoolers moving and eating healthfully. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior 36(6), 327-328.

Harnack, L. J., Oakes, J. M., French, S. A., Rydell, S. A., Farah, F. M., & Taylor, G. L.  (2012). Results from an experimental trial at a Head Start center to evaluate two meal service approaches to increase fruit and vegetable intake of preschool aged children. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition & Physical Activity, 9:51. Retrieved from
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/ pmc/articles/PMC3419662/pdf/1479-5868-9-51.pdf

Juonala, M., Juhola, J., Magnussen, C. G., Würtz, P., Viikari, J. S., Thomson, R., …Raitakari, O.T.  (2011). Childhood environmental and genetic predictors of adulthood obesity: The cardiovascular risk in young Finns study. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 96(9), E1542-1549. doi:10.1210/jc.2011-1243

Lobstein, T., Baur, L., & Uauy, R. (2004). Obesity in children and young people: A crisis in public health.Obesity Reviews, 5 (Suppl 1), S4-85. doi:10.1111/j1467-789X.2004.00133.x

Niemeier B., Tande D., Hwang J., Stastny S., & Hektner J. M. (2010).  Using education, exposure and environments to increase preschool children’s knowledge about fruits and vegetables. Journal of Extension, 48(1), 1-5.  Retrieved from
http://www.joe.org/joe/2010 february/iw6.php

Northstone, K. & Emmett, P. M. (2008). Are dietary patterns stable throughout early and mid-childhood? A birth cohort study. British Journal of Nutrition, 100(5), 1069-1076. doi:10.1017/S0007114508968264

Ogden, C. L., Carroll, M. D., Kit, B. K., & Flegal, K. M. (2012). Prevalence of obesity and trends in Body Mass Index among U.S. children and adolescents, 1999-2010. Journal of the American Medical Association, 307(5), 483-490. doi:10.1001/jama.2012.40

Robert Wood Johnson Foundation Health Policy Snapshot Childhood Obesity. (2012). Declining childhood obesity rates-Where are we seeing the most progress?  Retrieved from http://www.rwjf.org/content/dam/farm/reports/issue_briefs/2012/rwjf401163

Sigman-Grant, M., Christiansen, E., Fernandez, G., Fletcher, J., Johnson, S. L., Branen, L., & Price, B.A. (2011). Child care provider training and a supportive feeding environment in child care settings in 4 states, 2003. Preventing Chronic Disease, 8(5), A113. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/pcd/issues/2011/sep/10_0224.htm

Starc, G., & Sterl, J. (2010). Tracking excess weight and obesity from childhood to young adulthood: A 12-year prospective cohort study in Slovenia.  Public Health Nutrition, 14(1), 49-55. doi:10.1017/S1368980010000741

U.S. Department of Agriculture (2012).  National standards in the National School Lunch and School Breakfast Programs (Report). Washington, DC: USDA, Food & Nutrition Service.  Retrieved from http://www.gpo.gov/fdsys/pkg/FR-2012-01-26/pdf/2012-1010.pdf

Williamson, D. A., Han, H. Johnson, D. J., Martin, C. K., & Newton, R. L.  (2013). Modification of the school cafeteria environment can impact childhood nutrition. Results from the Wise Mind and LA Health Studies. Appetite61(1), 77-84. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2012.11.002

Biography

Tande is an Assistant Professor of Nutrition and Dietetics at the University of North Dakota.  Stastny, Hektner, and Habedank are, respectively, Associate Professor of Health, Nutrition and Exercise Sciences, Associate Professor in Human Development and Family Science, and director of the Center for child Development, all at North Dakota State University.  Niemeier is Assistant Professor in Health, Physical Education, Recreation and Coaching at the University of Wisconsin-Whitewater.  Hyunjoo Hwang is Assistant Professor of Travel Industry Management at the University of Hawaii, and Nandita Bezbaruah is Assistant Professor of Nursing and Healthcare Leadership at Minnesota State University Moorhead

Purpose / Objectives

The purpose of this pilot study was to compare changes in preschool children’s identification, preferences, and beliefs related to fruits and vegetables introduced to a child care center’s menu before and after a nutrition education and food exposure intervention.  The study also sought to determine how these changes were related to Body Mass Index (BMI).